Introduction
Wikipedia shows the following definition for the term
absolute pitch:
"Generally, absolute pitch implies some or all of the following abilities, achieved without a reference tone:
• Identify by name individual
pitches (e.g. F♯, A, G, C)
played on various instruments.
• Name the key of a given piece of tonal music.
• Reproduce a piece of tonal music in the correct key days after
hearing it.
• Identify and name all the tones of a given chord or other tonal
mass.
• Accurately sing a named pitch.
• Name the pitches of common everyday sounds such as car horns and
alarms.
• Name the frequency of a pitch (e.g. that G♯4 is 415Hz) after hearing it."
Unfortunately, the definition “some or all” is confusing and
leaves room for interpretation. A simpler definition given at the beginning of
the Wikipedia article says "Absolute pitch (AP), widely referred to
as perfect pitch,
is a rare auditory phenomenon characterized by the ability of a person to
identify or recreate a given musical note without the benefit of a reference
tone." This sentence is also ambiguous since it also contains an "or."
This ambiguity makes it very difficult – if not impossible –
to answer this question with a "Yes, you have it" or "No, you don’t have it."
This situation is aggravated by the fact that the Wikipedia’s definition
is not precise and no other official definition exists. For example, an individual might
be able to recognize notes in a very limited range or is capable of naming
simple chords, yet may fail to identify very complex chord names because
of the lack of knowledge in music theory. Or, how much time is allowed for recognizing a note or a chord name? Must the answer be given within a second, or is figuring it out in
a minute still accepted to count as an absolute pitch possessor?
On the other hand, how important is it to know if you have
absolute pitch or not?
Of course, having the ability to identify absolute pitches is fascinating.
However, music lives from change. That is, pitch changes relative to other
notes. Absolute pitch associates a name to a particular frequency, thus is
isolated. Creating harmony in music involves relations to other notes.
Being able to recognize notes, intervals, chords, and keys
requires knowledge of the underlying musical system. Detecting keys or chords
requires several notes; detecting intervals requires two notes. These notes are
then set in relation to each other. The underlying music system assigns these
constellations interval names, chord names, or a key name. Therefore, these
abilities have more to do with music theory and relative pitch skills than with
absolute pitch abilities.
This is also the main reason why it is generally accepted
that relative pitch is more important than absolute pitch. Many well-known
musicians did not have absolute pitch. Or maybe they just did not care about
it because harmony in music is always relative, and absolute pitch frequencies
are irrelevant for that matter.
Why, then, should we have an Absolute Pitch
indicator?
The only reason I can think of is to have a musical goal. Ear
training is an important aspect for any music student. There are our ears with
which we can judge if we are in tune with others or not. If we are playing
a solo, the ability to keep a fundamental note over a longer period in
our mind is a useful skill: it prevents us from drifting off the key.
Not having a single, continuous indicator for absolute pitch
makes it difficult to bring absolute pitch in line with a training program. The
road to absolute pitch may be long, and only a "Yes, you have it" or "No,
you don’t have it" answer does not leave room for a step-by-step progress.
To make gradual progress, we need better feedback. We can
break up the Wikipedia criteria for absolute pitch in separate tasks and
measure progress in each category. However, we still have to guess our progress
toward absolute pitch. More importantly, how do we measure the criterion: "identify
a pitch without a reference tone"?
The main problem here is "without a reference tone." If we
are given a reference pitch and then listen to a piece of music, chances are that we still can remember the reference tone after the piece has finished.
This is because we stay in a musical context. Therefore, we would solve absolute
pitch questions after the piece has finished still through relative pitch
skills.
To overcome this problem, we need to define a musical
absence time, before we can answer absolute pitch questions. Tests have shown
that after a quarter of an hour of musical absence, in most cases, the reference
tone has vanished – is no longer accessible through our short-term memory.
Now, this restriction of a mandatory musical absence makes
it nearly impossible to develop a test for testing the absolute pitch skill
“recognizing a pitch without a reference tone”
Even so, we have good possibilities for measuring the other
criteria for absolute pitch; we are still missing a method for measuring pitch
retention abilities. Since “without a reference tone” is where people have most
difficulties, the task of an absolute pitch indicator will be providing a pitch
retention metric.
Directly measuring retention abilities is difficult since
any test taken will interrupt the retention process. Thus, you would have to
start measuring with a very large time, and if you fail then you
would have to restart the test with a decreased time period until a successful
feedback is generated. Such a test would be very long and therefore unpractical.
However, we can speed
up this measurement if we can measure how pitch retention deteriorated during
a specified time. To develop a continuous indicator, we must depart from the
idea that we can only measure absolute pitch ability without a reference tone, if that "without" means a musical absence of fifteen minutes
or more. By testing at lower time periods, we can get a picture of pitch
retention deterioration.
Measuring how long you can retain a pitch with precision
will complete a musical test. Being able to retain a pitch for a longer time has many positive implications on musicality. Therefore, an absolute
pitch indicator still has its justification in a musical evaluation process.
Trying to improve this indicator is a valid musical goal, even if we do not
achieve absolute pitch as defined by Wikipedia.
Idea and development
In Wikipedia, the term perfect
pitch is redirected to absolute pitch.
This bringing together of the two terms is unfortunate. Perfect suggests that no errors are allowed. Perfect pitch implies many other abilities
that are related to pitch. This implication has led to the definition of absolute pitch that there is no simple
way of testing this ability. As a consequence, numerous rumors about absolute pitch have emerged. The biggest
rumor is that you cannot acquire it: you must be born with absolute pitch. Since the requirements are not clearly defined,
some people will not accept progress towards absolute pitch and will speak of
pseudo absolute pitch, if for example; someone learns to identify pitches after
singing a reference tone. They argue: It takes too long: it must be immediate. It
must be magic. Unfortunately, this "Yes" or "No”"view is not helping people to
see absolute pitch as a goal worth pursuing. Accepting imperfection and using a
continuous indicator could make absolute pitch training a challenge and lead to
a better ear.
The assumption that absolute pitch can be
learned like foreign languages has led to the development of the program Listening Ear Trainer. Both absolute pitch and foreign languages use voice to express
an idea. Both rely on retaining sound patterns in memory for references and
analyses. Therefore, it stands to reason that absolute pitch can be learned the
same way foreign languages can. The product Listening-Ear-Trainer (March
2013) introduced the learning box—used for vocabulary training—for learning
absolute pitches. Keeping an eye on the precise pitch (singing) reproduction improves the retention skills and confidence in your musical discrimination
ability.
Since no silence period between exercises in Listening-Ear-Trainer is mandatory, the training is not really for absolute but
relative pitch. Therefore, in August 2015, I released the program TuneCrack. A
direct application of keeping a pitch in mind is during the tuning process of
an instrument. Therefore the idea was: Cracking the tuning problem by giving
people a training program to increase their pitch retention skills.
The program TuneCrack recommends internalizing a pitch by
taking it over to your voice. However, singing is not mandatory since only the
precision-identifying ability is tested.
Since I believe that singing is the closest way you can get to
music, I searched for a method to get a reliable way of measuring what I think
is the most important criterion of absolute pitch: accurately sing a named pitch without a reference
tone. This criterion includes singing and accurate pitch retention. The result
was the introduction of the program SamePitchPlease (May 2016).
To improve the pitch retention training, the programs TuneCrack and SamePitchPlease introduced a silence period. However, these
two programs are training programs that guide you through incremental exercises
to better performance. If we want a tool that measures an individual’s absolute pitch
ability in a more general way, then we have to build an absolute pitch
meter that can be evaluated through a single test.
Restricting the program SamePitchPlease to four notes and a
25-cent deviation led to the Pitch Ability Test (November 2016), which returns an indicator on the pitch retention and reproduction ability. Both
abilities are required for absolute pitch; therefore, the indicator can be used
as a continuous measure for absolute pitch instead of only a "Yes, you have
it" or "No, you don’t have it" answer.
Defining the test and metrics
The Pitch Ability Test defines the ability to reproduce a
given pitch after a specified time period within a frequency precision of 25
cents as the measure. Thus, the Pitch Ability Test fixes the note accuracy for
the reproduction and uses only the time as an indicator.
As with any test, the more precise a result should be, the
more time is needed to determine the result. Therefore, a careful selection
between time and accuracy should be considered. In the Pitch Ability Test, a
decision to keep a single evaluation below 20 minutes was made. This limits the
number of tones that can be tested during that time frame. Since the test
should also prove that you can locate non-tested tones over a larger range, the
test requires four different notes. Having four notes as fixed points ensures that
you can orientate yourself by building distances between the tested notes and the directions: up or down.
The restriction of keeping a single test below 20 minutes
leads to a maximum of 4 minutes for each note. That is, the best result you can
get for the Pitch Ability Test is 4 minutes. While longer times can be easily
measured, increasing the time does not make sense. Most people will have
difficulty reaching this 4-minute limit—without training anyway. However, to
make it clear that you have reached this 4-minute limit, and probably can
reproduce pitches way beyond this point, the 4-minute point has a special name:
Felix’s Pitch Point.
The result of the Pitch Ability Test is the silence time
between the sounding of the reference sound and the accurate reproduction of
the pitch with your voice. Pitch ability is expressed in seconds.
The test procedure
The requirements for administering the test are a stopwatch
and a person with a good ear. This person does not need to have
absolute pitch. Playing the correct note on an instrument and comparing the
pitch with the sung note can evaluate accuracy.
The test procedure is as follows:
- The person who wants to test his or her ability tells the tester four different notes that he or she thinks he or she can sing well and announces the period of silence for which he or she wants to take the test.
- The tester plays the first note for about two seconds.
- The tester starts the stopwatch.
- The person taking the test is allowed to sing back the note immediately for about two seconds. No feedback is given.
- The tester monitors whether no pitched sounds can be heard during the following period of silence.
- About five seconds before the announced test time is due, the tester signals to the candidate (with a visual sign or non-pitched sound) that he or she should prepare for the singing back of the note.
- When the time is due, the candidate should sing the note steadily for about a second. The tester listens carefully, compares the sung pitch with the original pitch, and decides whether the pitch did not deviate more than 25 cents from the original pitch.
- The tester repeats steps 2 to 7 for the remaining three notes.
- If the candidate succeeds with all four notes, then he or she has fulfilled the Pitch Ability Test for the chosen period of silence.
To determine pitch ability, the test uses an unconventional
approach: the retention time gets measured in a similar way as the height in
high-jump sports gets measured. This approach allows you to measure the
retention time of the pitch with enough precision. If you select a low-enough
time, you will always pass. If you come to your limits, the results will start
to vary. Reaching Felix’s Pitch Point is not easy, but to acquire absolute pitch
(or perfect pitch), you must pass this point. Therefore, the Pitch Ability Test
is a useful tool for giving you feedback on where you stand on the road to absolute
pitch. Most people who want to acquire absolute pitch are far away from Felix’s
Pitch Point; thus, they can improve their ability.
A computer-based
implementation
Since the wait—until you are allowed to reproduce the pitch—is
very boring for the tester and the determination of the pitch accuracy is often
non-neutral, a computer-based implementation of the procedure was made. The
program Pitch Ability Test executes the above-described procedure and
automatically suggests that you take a test for a higher time limit if you
passed all four notes. This procedure is similar to that in high-jump sports,
where you set your targeted goal and continue until you fail. Therefore, you
should select your silence period with care. Starting with a very short period
may fatigue and hinder you from reaching your best performance. Starting with a
very long period poses the risk of getting no result at all.
Next steps
Please let me know what you think about my proposed absolute
pitch indicator/absolute pitch meter. Do you know other approaches for
measuring absolute pitch? Or do you have ideas for improvements?
Keine Kommentare:
Kommentar veröffentlichen